Cover photo

From Genius Science to Scenius Science

Why personal AI tools might not be all we need to revolutionize science

(cover image from Matt Akamatsu's excellent talk at DeSci Denver 2024)

Hello sensemakers!

We’ve been hard at work over the past few months preparing the first alpha release of Sensemaking Networks. We’ll share more updates about our progress soon, but this will kick off a series of posts where we will share some of our learnings as we’re engaging with the decentralized/open science research communities around the project.

For those who haven’t seen the previous episodes, Sensemaking Networks (or SenseNets) is a collective intelligence system designed to radically enhance information sharing and collaborative knowledge synthesis among researchers by integrating decentralized semantic web technology with AI and social networks. In our work on SenseNets over the past few months, we have come to notice a puzzle at the heart of how we do science.

The puzzle, which we will explore in this post, is that many scientists value collaboration with other researchers, but the tools and metrics we use isolate us and promote competition. Why is that?

To feel into this tension, imagine you are doing a literature survey of a new field, and have a stack of research papers in front of you. You have to decide which ones to read and in what order. To help you decide, you have access to assessments of each paper, but you need to choose between GPT-generated assessments or human assessments made by peers and experts in their field. Which would you choose?

I asked the audience this question during a presentation of Sensemaking Networks at an Open Science night organized by Astera in May. I was surprised by how few people preferred the GPT option (hey, this is the Bay Area, after all 🙃).

These two choices (GPT and peer assessments) represent two different approaches to sensemaking in science: the former is focused on empowering individual researchers in isolation, while the latter focuses on empowering collective, networked and social aspects of sensemaking.

Sure, phrasing the question as a binary (either one or the other) is a little unfair. Indeed, we are most excited by synergistic combinations of both approaches. However, the story is a useful demonstration of the puzzle we opened with: people actually really value social sensemaking, and yet technology is overwhelmingly tailored to individual sensemaking. Personal AI research assistants are a dime a dozen, while tools that help researchers share information and think better together are few and far between. The very lack of recognition for the role of social media in science is the raison d'être of Sensemaking Networks.

In fact, this isn’t just the tech focus: academic research itself is also primarily tailored towards augmenting individual researchers. Recent research even proposed developing a personalized “science exocortex” using a swarm of AI agents to automate an ever-growing range of scientific activities while leaving just the highest level decisions for the human operator.

What’s gives? Why are we so focused on the individual side of science? And, given that we intuitively really value working with other humans, why don’t we have more tools that improve collective sensemaking?

It turns out that there are a lot of intriguing answers to these questions. We’ll take a whirlwind tour through sociology, meta-science and collective intelligence theory, and reflect on what these diverse strands of research might imply for the future of SenseNets and science information systems more broadly.

Sociology and lone genius mythology

Part of the answer lies in the “lone genius mythology” around science. We were all raised on stories of genius scientists who defied their doubters and against all odds made discoveries that advanced the frontier of human knowledge. These stories weren’t wrong, of course; these scientists were extraordinarily talented and determined individuals. Yet the stories are also incomplete, for example omitting reference to other crucial (e.g., social) factors that contributed to the process of discovery. An insightful critique of the recent Hollywood portrayal of Robert Oppenheimer contrasts Hollywood’s “lone genius and his blackboard science” with a perspective inspired by French sociologist Bruno Latour, according to whom “’The father of the atomic bomb’ was no single parent, but rather a collective, networked one…“

In other words, the developers of sensemaking tools for individual researchers might be drawing more inspiration from Iron Man than from Latour.

But the focus on individual researchers is not just a product of the stories we are told about scientists; there are more pervasive forces at play.

Meta-science: Re-thinking the pecking order in science

A thought-provoking recent paper called “Shifting the Level of Selection in Science” shows how an overfocus on individuals is built into the very fabric of science, by way of the reward structures used to evaluate scientists:

The predominant approach to scientific evaluation uses individual-level criteria, such as one’s number of first-authored publications, citations, h-indices, journal impact factors, and success in funding acquisition… This evaluation strategy implicitly assumes that identifying and rewarding the most accomplished individuals is the best way to generate scientific knowledge.

The authors show how individual-level criteria encourage competition and personally beneficial behavior among scientists. But, as they observe,

personally beneficial behaviors are only a subset of the behaviors that benefit science. For example, the scientific community plausibly benefits from the open sharing of information such as code, materials, and raw data, whereas individual-level competition disincentivizes information-sharing to hinder competitors’ success

It turns out that we have a lot to learn from chicken farmers:

the most productive hens in a coop are also the nastiest hens, feather-pecking and cannibalizing the other hens in their coop. Because individual hens who are most productive are those that harm others, selectively breeding the most productive hens can actually lead to lower overall egg production

Instead of selecting the most productive individuals, breeders have learned to select the most productive groups.

Perhaps science can do the same? The authors make a solid case for expanding evaluation mechanisms to incentivize group-level outcomes in addition to individual-level metrics. Importantly, such evaluation will require far greater recognition of diverse prosocial contributions, such as information sharing and other currently invisible “team science” roles.

So we have another piece of the puzzle - in a world where researchers are being incentivized to conceal rather than share their insights, private AI assistants are a safer bet for tool developers, even though social sharing platforms might actually be more effective in advancing science.

Science as process, not product

Another fascinating line of research goes even more existential, in questioning the very aim of science. “Shifting the Level of Selection in Science” proposed new group-level incentives, but still focuses on a productivity-based model where science is about producing new knowledge. A recent piece called “An Epistemology for Democratic Citizen Science” makes a compelling case that sometimes the goal is the journey in science. Rather than just viewing the knowledge as the product of science (what they call “industrial science”), they make the case for re-thinking the social and cognitive processes that are generating scientific knowledge (”ecological science”). Ecological science, beyond traditional science’s role as an inquiry into the natural world, is also “an inquiry into how to best cultivate and utilise humanity’s collective intelligence”.

On this view, the social tools we use to communicate about science would themselves be at the core of the scientific process. Those tools would function as large scale experiments in collective intelligence (CI), informed by the latest research. Science Twitter is cool, but leaves much to be desired as a tool for researchers. We have a lot more to learn from CI theory; in “Science Communication as a Collective Intelligence Endeavor”, the authors provide an outline of what CI systems for science might look like. In particular, such systems would (a) enable better aggregation of distributed knowledge, (b) involve a more diverse group of contributors and (c) encourage increased public participation in science.

Connecting this research to our opening question, perhaps many tool developers are building “industrial science” tools for enhancing individual productivity, as opposed to “ecological science” tools for enhancing collective intelligence.

Industrial science, like industrial agriculture, risks creating scientific monocultures where

some types of methods, questions and viewpoints come to dominate alternative approaches, making science less innovative and more vulnerable to errors.

In contrast, ecological science is more like permaculture, reflecting a diversity of methods, contributions and perspectives. See Matt Akamatsu’s talk at DeSci Denver for an exciting example of what this might look like

Conclusion: from Genius Science to Scenius Science

For those of you who haven’t heard of the term “scenius”, musician-activist Brian Eno coined it some 30 years ago:

I became (and still am) more and more convinced that the important changes in cultural history were actually the product of very large numbers of people and circumstances conspiring to make something new. I call this ‘scenius’ - it means ‘the intelligence and intuition of a whole cultural scene’. It is the communal form of the concept of genius.

Curiously, scenius has been around for a while but actually hasn’t taken off:

But given the dominant focus on individuals that we’ve seen, maybe we shouldn’t be too surprised at this point 🤷

Nonetheless, it feels like genius is fading and scenius is in the kairos. Converging evidence from across multiple fields suggests that if we’re really serious about enhancing science for the benefit of humanity, we should be thinking less about how to find geniuses and a lot more about how to create sceniuses.

This post was inspired by the Sensemaking Scenius, our own scenius in the making 🏗

Sensemaking Networks logo
Subscribe to Sensemaking Networks and never miss a post.